Chapter One - The Planning Fallacy: Cognitive, Motivational, and Social Origins

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(10)43001-4Get rights and content

Abstract

The planning fallacy refers to a prediction phenomenon, all too familiar to many, wherein people underestimate the time it will take to complete a future task, despite knowledge that previous tasks have generally taken longer than planned. In this chapter, we review theory and research on the planning fallacy, with an emphasis on a programmatic series of investigations that we have conducted on this topic. We first outline a definition of the planning fallacy, explicate controversies and complexities surrounding its definition, and summarize empirical research documenting the scope and generality of the phenomenon. We then explore the origins of the planning fallacy, beginning with the classic inside–outside cognitive model developed by Kahneman and Tversky [Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Intuitive prediction: biases and corrective procedures. TIMS Studies in Management Science, 12, 313–327]. Finally, we develop an extended inside–outside model that integrates empirical research examining cognitive, motivational, social, and behavioral processes underlying the planning fallacy.

Section snippets

Defining the Planning Fallacy

The planning fallacy refers to a readily observable phenomenon: the conviction that a current project will go as well as planned even though most projects from a relevant comparison set have failed to fulfill their planned outcomes. The term was first introduced to the psychological literature by Kahneman & Tversky, 1979, Kahneman & Tversky, 1982a, p. 415) to describe people's tendency “to underestimate the time required to complete a project, even when they have considerable experience of past

Documenting the Planning Fallacy

To identify examples of the planning fallacy requires two related but separable findings. First, predictions of current task completion times must be more optimistic than beliefs about the distribution of past completion times for similar projects; second, predictions of current task completion times must be more optimistic than actual outcomes. A high degree of confidence is also diagnostic, but is not required by the classic definition.

The Standish Group publishes an annual survey about the

The inside versus outside view

Given the prevalence of optimistic predictions, and ample empirical evidence of the planning fallacy, we now turn to examining the psychological mechanisms that underlie people's optimistic forecasts. In particular, how do people segregate their general theories about their predictions (i.e., that they are usually unrealistically optimistic) from their specific expectations for an upcoming task? Kahneman and Tversky (1979) explained the prediction failure of the curriculum development team

Empirical Support for the Inside–Outside Model

An important goal of our initial studies of the planning fallacy was to test the inside–outside account. We first looked for evidence that a focus on the future—and a consequent neglect of the past—was indeed characteristic of the planning process. In several studies, we included “think-aloud” procedures to record the “online” narratives of participants as they estimated their completion times for various tasks, most of which had specific deadlines. We instructed respondents to say aloud every

Identifying key elements of the inside focus

A basic axiom of the planning fallacy is that the future is perceived to be rosier than the past; realistically pessimistic lessons from the past fade from a forecaster's attention in light of optimistic plans about the future. However, according to Kahneman and Tversky's (1979) initial definition, the inside view includes all case-specific content including possible problems or obstacles to completion as well as plans for how to overcome them. Thus, an inside view can, at least in theory, vary

Concluding Perspectives

The planning fallacy is a phenomenon rooted in daily life and daily experience, rich enough to keep our research group busy for the best part of two decades. Although this chapter explored the planning fallacy in the domain of task completion predictions, we have also documented similar patterns and psychological processes in people's tendency to overpredict the longevity of their romantic relationships (Buehler et al., 1995; see also MacDonald & Ross, 1999) and to overestimate the intensity of

Acknowledgments

This program of research was supported by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

References (137)

  • V.B. Hinsz et al.

    Using judgmental anchors to establish challenging self-set goals without jeopardizing commitment

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

    (1997)
  • D. Kahneman et al.

    Variants of uncertainty

    Cognition

    (1982)
  • D. Koehler et al.

    Self-predictions overweight strength of current intentions

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • J. Kruger et al.

    If you don't want to be late, enumerate: Unpacking reduces the planning fallacy

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • N. Liberman et al.

    The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2002)
  • V.G. Morwitz et al.

    The mere-measurement effect: Why does measuring intentions change actual behavior

    Journal of Consumer Psychology

    (2004)
  • D. Adams et al.

    Last chance to see

    (1991)
  • D. Ariely et al.

    Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: Self-control by precommitment

    Psychological Science

    (2002)
  • H.R. Arkes et al.

    Eliminating the hindsight bias

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (1988)
  • D.A. Armor et al.

    Accuracy, error, and bias in predictions for real versus hypothetical events

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • D.A. Armor et al.

    The effects of mindset on behavior: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2003)
  • J.S. Armstrong

    Long-range forecasting: From crystal ball to computer

    (1985)
  • A. Bandura

    Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change

    Psychological Review

    (1977)
  • F.P. Brooks

    The mythical man-month: Essays on software engineering

    (1975)
  • M.K. Brunnermeier et al.

    An economic model of the planning fallacy

    (2008)
  • R. Buehler et al.

    Motivated prediction in observers

    (2009)
  • R. Buehler et al.

    Intensity bias in affective forecasting: The role of temporal focus

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2001)
  • R. Buehler et al.

    The role of motivated reasoning in optimistic time predictions

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1997)
  • R. Buehler et al.

    Exploring the “planning fallacy”: Why people underestimate their task completion times

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1994)
  • R. Buehler et al.

    It's about time: Optimistic predictions in work and love

  • R. Buehler et al.

    Inside the planning fallacy: The causes and consequences of optimistic time predictions

  • R. Buehler et al.

    Culture and optimism: The planning fallacy in Japan and North America

    (2003)
  • R. Buehler et al.
  • C.D.B. Burt et al.

    Construction of activity duration and time management potential

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (1994)
  • S.J. Byram

    Cognitive and motivational factors influencing time predictions

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied

    (1997)
  • C.S. Carver et al.

    On the self-regulation of behavior

    (1998)
  • D. Cervone et al.

    Anchoring, efficacy, and action: The influence of judgmental heuristics on self-efficacy judgments and behavior

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • T. Connolly et al.

    Decomposed versus holistic estimates of effort required for software writing tasks

    Management Science

    (1997)
  • R.M. Dawes

    Rational choice in an uncertain world

    (1988)
  • J. Deslauriers

    Should we plan for upcoming tasks? Effects of planning on prediction accuracy for simple and difficult assignments

    (2002)
  • D. Dunning

    Prediction: The inside view

  • S.L. Engerman et al.

    Digging the dirt at public expense: Governance in the building of the Erie Canal and other public works

    (2004)
  • T. Eyal et al.

    The pros and cons of temporally near and distant action

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • L. Festinger

    A theory of cognitive dissonance

    (1957)
  • B. Fischhoff et al.

    Fault trees: Sensitivity of estimated failure probabilities to problem representation

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1978)
  • B. Flyvbjerg

    Curbing optimism bias and strategic misrepresentation in planning: Reference class forecasting in practice

    European Planning Studies

    (2008)
  • B. Flyvbjerg et al.

    Megaprojects and risk: An anatomy of ambition

    (2003)
  • B. Flyvbjerg et al.

    Delusion and deception in large infrastructure projects: Two models for explaining and preventing executive disaster

    California Management Review

    (2009)
  • B. Flyvbjerg et al.

    What causes cost overrun in transport infrastructure projects?

    Transport Reviews

    (2004)
  • B. Flyvbjerg et al.

    How (in)accurate are demand forecasts in public works projects?

    Journal of the American Planning Association

    (2005)
  • Cited by (85)

    • Moral inconsistency

      2023, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
    • Financial self-control strategy use: Generating personal strategies reduces spending more than learning expert strategies

      2021, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, spending goals were notably higher in Study 2 compared to Study 1. This may have been due to this difference in the order of manipulation and goal assessment, or this might be due to the fact that participants in Study 2 were asked to recall their past monthly spending directly before setting the goal (past behavior is most predictive of future behavior, e.g., Buehler, Griffin, & Peetz, 2010; Ouellette & Wood, 1998). Notably, this difference in goal amount led to a difference between studies in terms of the goal accuracy.

    • Task difficulty and overconfidence. Evidence from distance running

      2019, Journal of Economic Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Throughout the paper we discuss several reasons why we believe this type of artefact has little bearing on our results as well, leaving a true psychological tendency as the most plausible explanation. Second, given that we focus on the predicted time required to complete a task (cover the distance), our project is clearly related to the planning fallacy literature, see Buehler, Griffin, and Peetz (2010) for a review). These studies seem to have a flavor of Murphy's law: every project takes (much) longer than expected, even when those forming expectations are aware of previous, similar experiences.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text